Lab Notes: What makes Sydney's cockies so clever?
News grab: Move over Ibis, there's a new bin chicken in town. The iconic Aussie bird's mostly been prying open bin lids.
Belinda Smith: And now, they're taking our water.
News grab: Sulphur crested cockatoos in Western Sydney have been observed using public drinking fountains, learning to twist the handles and drinking from the bubblers.
Belinda Smith: And it's not just one or two cockies doing this. More than 100 have been spotted drinking from bubblers. So, what do these entertaining exploits tell us about cocky innovation? And even about cocky culture? Hi, I'm Belinda Smith and you're listening to Lab Notes, the show that dissects the science behind new discoveries and current events. To explain why these birds are so bloody brainy is Lucy Aplin, a cognitive ecologist at the Australian National University. Now, I've seen children struggle to turn a water fountain tap and they have hands. Cockies have claws. So, how do they manage?
Lucy Aplin: What they have to do is they have to hold on to the stem of the fountain. So, imagine a fountain you might get in your local sports fields, very classic upright model. So, they're gripping onto the stem of the upright part with one foot and then they're turning the twist handle with their other foot. Now, the twist handle has a spring in it. So, it'll come back so that the water doesn't run forever. So, the birds actually have to put their weight down to hold that spring. And then they have to twist their head back to drink the water while keeping the weight over the handle. So, it's quite a complex action that actually involves every body part.
Belinda Smith: It's not just the strength to turn the tap but also the flexibility to kind of manoeuvre their body so that they can then enjoy the water that's coming out of the tap. Exactly, and
Lucy Aplin: the foot-mouth coordination. I always say eye-hand-eye coordination, but foot-eye coordination, foot-eye-mouth coordination in this case. So, where do these particular cockies live? So, we observed this behaviour in the Western Sydney Parklands area just near Nurragingy Reserve, which is around sort of near Doonside train station, if anyone knows Western Sydney.
Belinda Smith: In 2019, purely by chance, one of Lucy's colleagues, Barbara Klump, first spotted a thirsty cocky.
Lucy Aplin: She was walking across the sports field and she saw them doing this. And she then walked into the Nurragingy Reserve, which is next door, and asked the rangers about it. And they said, oh yeah, they do it around here all the time. They've been doing it for ages. So, I think they thought it was a, you know, this is just what the local cockies do. As many Australians will attest, they do lots of weird and wonderful things. But we were pretty excited, so we thought, right, we have to study this.
Belinda Smith: Then, not long after that initial sighting, Lucy and her team started keeping tabs on one particularly popular drinking fountain. The
Lucy Aplin: drinking fountain that was most used was the one in the sports field right next to where the sleeping trees were. So, they were using it first thing in the morning and in the afternoon. How successful are they at getting water? So, we found when we did these intensive observations at one drinking fountain, that actually only about just under 50 per cent of the attempts were successful. So, individuals could try multiple times. And sometimes the attempts were unsuccessful because they were obviously distracted by the other birds that were queuing. Maybe a more dominant bird was queuing and they were trying to keep an eye on that bird at the same time and that was influencing their success. So, they were aware that this thing could produce water and were trying and were either in the process of learning or were very successful at it already.
Belinda Smith: Ah, right. And so, did they learn from watching other cockies do it?
Lucy Aplin: That's what we assume. So, we don't think that every individual cocky innovated this behaviour or we would be equally likely to see cockatoos drinking from drinking fountains all over Australia. Everywhere there are cockatoos and drinking fountains of which there are many places with both. So, we think that the evidence we have is highly suggestive that actually it's spreading through social learning. So, one bird or maybe a couple of birds initially invented this behaviour, worked it out and then other birds observed them and adopted the behaviour and it spread through the local group to form a local tradition in this area. So,
Belinda Smith: when this behaviour was sort of first observed by your postdoc, Barbara, was this during a particularly dry
Lucy Aplin: period? No, it was actually towards the end of winter. So, it would have been in August, September because that's when we're usually doing our fieldwork. So, it wasn't particularly dry. It was something that we were a little bit surprised by for that exact reason. We thought this is a really interesting behaviour for the technical innovation part of it. It seems quite complex and it's interesting to understand how it spreads and whether it's continuing to spread. But alongside that, there's this really interesting question which is what is the adaptive benefit of it? Because we'd just come off studying another innovation which was the bin opening behaviour which we're still studying in the south of Sydney. And there, it's really obvious what the cockatoos are getting out of it. They're getting bread or pasta or pizza or all high calorie items. Delicious things. Exactly. But here, there's an artificial lake that they've made in the lovely Chinese gardens in the Nurragingy Parklands. There's a local river they could be using. There's lots of local water sources and yet they seem to be preferring to do this highly complicated behaviour where often they also have to queue for it. So, it was a bit of a mystery we also wanted to solve as to why. So, did you find out? Well, we have three working hypotheses. Unfortunately, we couldn't answer them in this study but we're hoping to get to them. So, in this study, we wanted to ask just straightforward, are they using this as a supplement to other water sources? For example, when it's really hot and dry. And we saw no evidence for that. It seemed more like they were actually preferring to use drinking fountains over those other alternative water sources. So, then we had three follow on hypotheses. Maybe the water just tastes better. And that's not, I think, out of this world because if you think about whether you would rather go down to the creek, drink slimy, silty, muddy creek water. Or whether you might prefer a nice Sydney tap water, even though some people might be rude about Sydney tap water. It's probably still preferable to muddy creek water. So, that is a possibility that they're also making that choice. The other potential explanation we thought about is whether they just feel safer drinking from drinking fountains. Because they're usually, or all of these, in this case, were in picnic areas or they're on the edge of sports fields. So, they're in open areas with really good visibility. They're off the ground. And maybe that feels much safer than going down to that waterhole where maybe the predators are lurking behind the bush.
Belinda Smith: Part of me also wonders if there's some payoff to actually working for the water. It makes me think of shelling pistachios. Somehow, the pistachio tastes much better if you're the one cracking it open. And it's like that little bit of work has just made the reward so much more tasty.
Lucy Aplin: Yes. That exact phenomenon you're describing does have a scientific name. You know, there's jargon for everything. Yes, great. It's called contra-freeloading. And it means that you'd rather work a little bit for your reward than get it for free. And it is something that has been observed in humans. And it's also been observed in captive parrots.
Belinda Smith: Okay, so we've got bin lid flipping cockies. We've got water fountain drinking cockies. What other behaviours have been reported in cockatoos?
Lucy Aplin: So one I find perhaps a little bit disturbing is that they seem to have taken a liking in some parts of Sydney to drinking discarded energy drink cans or soft drink cans. So if they find them on the ground or in the rubbish, they'll pick them up using their bill into the sort of hole, you know, the drinking hole and then tip it back to try and get the drugs.
Belinda Smith: Oh, my gosh. A caffeinated cocky. Exactly. Just what you need.
Lucy Aplin: That's why I find that one a little bit disturbing. I'm not sure what the effect of caffeine is on cockatoos, but I don't think it'd be good. And we have going along the same sort of high energy line, just like we have observations of noisy miners in Rainbow Larrakeet stealing sugar packets, cockatoos will also occasionally steal sugar packets. That's been reported to us. And another innovation which has been reported to us from the south of Sydney, from the Northern Territory, amazingly, from a little community right up in Arnhem Land and from a couple of sites up in far north of Sydney is what we're calling the lunch bag innovation. It's a bit hard to know how to label this one. It seems to involve cockatoos going to schools where the kids leave their bags outside the classroom, unzipping the bags, taking out the lunch boxes and then undoing the lunch boxes and running off with the sandwiches.
Belinda Smith: Oh, that's so cheeky. OK, so why and how can these cockies do such amazing and often annoying things?
Lucy Aplin: There's a few hypotheses that have been put forward. When we look at overall brain size across parrots, it seems like one thing that fits really well is what we call the cognitive buffer hypothesis. And it's this idea that if you live a long time, which parrots do, your environment's going to be changing around you, unless you're in a really stable environment. But parrots are often not in a really stable environment. The environment's changing and having a large brain allows them to cope with that change that they might experience during their life. So it provides a buffer to the sort of swings and arrows of fortune by allowing you to work out solutions to changes. And that sort of explanation for the evolution of intelligence in parrots, it's not the only one, but that hypothesis fits really nicely with the urban adaptability that we see because urban environments are that. They're changing, novel environments where things can change. They can be pretty stable, but then they can change really rapidly when the local government decides to change all of the drinking fountains or whatever. So having a big brain might allow you to cope with that by learning new solutions and adjusting your behaviour.
Belinda Smith: Wow. Just like us humans then. And cockies don't just have big brains. They also have a lot of cells or neurons in those brains.
Lucy Aplin: So a cockatoo has about the same total number of neurons. So just straight count, not accounting for differences in body size or anything, just straight count. The same total number of neurons or similar to a macaque monkey, which is a medium sized monkey. Wow. So they're really punching above their weight. Like literally?
Belinda Smith: Yes. These behaviours, they're kind of quirky and fun and maybe a bit messy, but they're very interesting. But what does this tell us about cocky culture?
Lucy Aplin: Well, it tells us that they're very capable of learning from each other, from social learning. We have no evidence for teaching, so they're not doing that. Watch out if they start. No evidence for that. But they are very capable of observing other individuals and adopting the behaviour if it's beneficial. And they're capable of transmitting that behaviour over quite large geographic areas. Some of the studies we're doing now, they're not published yet, so I don't have the full set of results. But our preliminary results from experimental work is showing that actually new beneficial behaviours can spread over, say, the entire city of Canberra within just a couple of weeks. So the way their society is organised is really promotes that rapid spread and population level adoption of new behaviours. So I think that tells us something about the secret to their success and something about their life history in general. These are the elephants of the bird world. They're really long-lived, they're really brainy, and they're really social. And so they have a society that we recognise and we can also understand. We can look at what they're doing and understand it better than with some other species.
Belinda Smith: That was Lucy Aplin, a cognitive ecologist at the Australian National University. She and her colleagues published their study about the water fountain drinking cockies in the Royal Society Biology Letters last week. And thanks for listening to Lab Notes on ABC Radio National, where every week we dissect the science behind new discoveries and current events. I'm Belinda Smith. This episode was produced on the lands of the Wurundjeri and Menang Noongar people. Fiona Pepper's the producer and it was mixed by Angie Grant. We'll catch you next week.
Hashtags

Try Our AI Features
Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:
Comments
No comments yet...
Related Articles

ABC News
an hour ago
- ABC News
Smartphones affect young people's mental health so are dumbphones the answer?
How old were you when you got your first smartphone? For many Aussies born in the past two generations, such as Ekaager, 16, Kian, 15, Daiwik, 16, and Jiujiu, 15, smartphones have been a part of life since early childhood. "I think I was 12 or 11, around that age," Ekaager said. Kian said: "I think I was like 10 or 11." "I was definitely 12," Daiwik said. "Yeah, something like 12," Jiujiu said. A global 2024 study commissioned by tech company Human Mobile Devices (HMD) found that young people were being given smartphones at an average age of 11 years old. In some cases, smartphones were being introduced as young as three years old. This early introduction to smartphones has "dramatically reshaped childhood and adolescence", according to new research published in the Journal of Human Development and Capabilities. Using survey data from more than 100,000 18 to 24-year-olds, researchers found that the younger those surveyed got a smartphone, from the age of 13 and under, the worse their mental health was. The specific symptoms reported included suicidal thoughts, aggression and detachment from reality. When asked what they thought about the findings, the teens BTN High spoke to said it made sense. "Especially because a lot of the platforms I use, like Instagram, can be a little addictive … it's hard for me to put it down," Lily, 15, said. "I remember in year 10 I was really at my lowest and I think that social media was the significant contributing factor to it," Eric, 17, said. Harsimrat, 15, said it was not just the mental health impacts. "There are many issues like attention span," he said. "Because even myself, sometimes in class, I just doze off because I just can't spend that much time looking at the board." Child psychologist Michael Carr-Gregg led a review into phone use in schools in 2018, which has since resulted in a nationwide ban on phones in public schools. Dr Carr-Gregg said that while the study's results were new, it was something experts had suspected. "I think it was 2007, the Apple iPhone was launched, we started replacing a play-based childhood with a screen-based childhood," he said. "And right across the western world … we have seen an increase in depression and anxiety and self-harm. "Therefore, this is just a confirmation of what we already knew." The study also recommends "graduated access restrictions," keeping anyone under 13 from having a smartphone, which Dr Carr-Gregg approves of. "Smartphones tend to be a distraction. They do interfere with sleep," he said. "They do create, I think, avenues for cyberbullying, extortion, those sorts of things. "So, for me, the result is from the university of the bleeding obvious, which is why I think a restriction, a delay, on young people having smartphones is sensible." Some of the teens BTN High spoke to also agreed it was a good idea. Lachlan, 15, said: "It's just probably for the better in future life." Others, like Angus, 16, weren't keen on it. "Thirteen is past that age where we … enter high school," he said. "I feel like phones are a big part of socialising and that sort of connection that many people rely on. "So I think it's a bit harsh." A "dumbphone", also known as a feature phone, is a basic device with limited functionality. No internet browsing, no apps, no complex multimedia functions – and that's the appeal for some young people, such as 18-year-old Jameson Butler. She is the co-founder of The Luddite Club, a group of teens in New York dedicated to promoting the conscious consumption of technology. "My sleep schedule was off the charts," Jameson said. "I was staying up until 3am. I was staying in my room, you know, on my phone. "And I just felt bad … I didn't feel there were any options or ways to combat that." Jameson said after ditching her smartphone, life improved. "I'm more present at family dinners. I do my schoolwork faster, I have better grades, I've become a lot less scatterbrained, and I've just become a lot more driven and ambitious." The Luddite Club has been gaining popularity. Jameson told the ABC's Brain Rot Podcast that she was seeing Luddite Clubs starting to pop up in Florida and Philadelphia, and she had even had some enquiries from Australia. While the dumbphone trend has been growing in the US and Europe, I want to find out if it is something young Aussies are doing too. After sitting at the University of Adelaide for more than an hour, no young people I spoke to even knew of anyone who used a dumbphone. "Yeah, this is too old," said one student. Harsimrat agreed, saying, "No, that's back in the old days." None of Dr Carr-Gregg's clients are on board either. "I can honestly put my hand on my heart and say I don't have one who's doing that," he said. "Once you've got a smartphone, I think the likelihood of you going back to a dumbphone is very, very small. "And that's because really the way in which my clients communicate with one another these days is through social media. "And therefore the smartphone is absolutely essential." Alex, 17, said a loss of connection might be putting young Aussies off. "I think there's a big fear of missing out," he said. "It's like if people aren't connected with their screens and social media and stuff, they … can't see what other people are posting, [what] they're doing on it. "A lot of my friends live in different places throughout the world, and I wouldn't be able to call them on a phone like that," Lily said. Despite social media being a major part of smartphone use, some teens, such as Alex, reckon Australia's incoming ban for under-16s won't change too many minds. "There are other things you can do on [smartphones], like games and stuff, even though you don't have social media," he said. Simar, 16, said in her case, a smartphone was handy for her parents to know her location because she had to walk to school by herself. Ekaager thinks young people will keep their smartphones and find ways around the ban. Nevertheless, Ekaager was open to giving the dumbphone trend a go, as was Eric. "Maybe I should with the current exam season coming up," Eric said. Lily agreed that it would be "a nice experiment to try". And Alex said, "I'd probably struggle to, but I would maybe give it a try just to improve my mental health."

ABC News
2 hours ago
- ABC News
What is Australia's most underrated animal? Vote for your favourite this National Science Week
In National Science Weeks of years gone by, you told us your favourite tree (the majestic river red gum) and the animal call Australia loves the most (the warbling magpie, for obvious reasons). This year, ABC Science is shining a spotlight on Australia's underrated animals. With the help of social media, experts and conservation organisations, we've found 10 incredible animals you may never have heard of, and probably don't know a whole lot about. From the slimy to the bitey and the downright weird — each of these creatures has traditionally been overshadowed by more charismatic species such as koalas and kangaroos. But we think it's time they had their day in the sun. Will it be the sharpshooting velvet worm, the mysterious marsupial mole or the secretive short-finned eel? Are you team rakali, dugong or the great desert skink? Or maybe you think the turtle frog, ghost bat, giant cuttlefish or palm cockatoo should win. Click on the cards below to find out more about each of these Australian faunal underdogs. The ghost bat is one of 10 underrated native animals people can vote for this National Science Week. ( ABC ) As one of Australia's only carnivorous bats, it's probably a good idea to not make fun of this mammal's giant ears and nose. The ghost bat ( Macroderma gigas ) hails from northern Queensland, Western Australia and the Northern Territory. Ghost bats live in colonies of up to 1,500 individuals, and roost in caves and old abandoned mine shafts. Ghost bats have a creche for looking after their young when the mothers go out hunting. ( Supplied: Bat Chat for Conservation ) According to Nicola Hanrahan, a bat ecologist from Charles Darwin University, ghost bats are both cute and ferocious. "They're a very silent flyer," Dr Hanrahan said. "People just wouldn't know they're out flying around, taking birds out of trees and landing on the ground to catch frogs." Their most distinctive feature is their snoot — an extremely large "nose leaf," which they use to push out and direct high-pitched noises for echolocation. They're Australia's largest microbat, but are they your underrated fave? If so, vote below! Alien: Giant cuttlefish The cuttlefish is one of 10 underrated native animals people can vote for this National Science Week. ( ABC ) While they can grow up to a metre long, the world's largest cuttlefish species can also disappear in a puff of ink and mucous. The giant cuttlefish ( Ascarosepion apama) is an oddball, and you can catch a glimpse of it around the whole bottom half of Australia — all the way from Brisbane to Shark Bay. But while most people might only know these spectacular creatures from the cuttlebones that regularly wash up on shore, Mark Norman, chief scientist at Parks Victoria, says there is a whole lot more to love about giant cuttlefish. "They've got a beak like a parrot with a tongue like a chainsaw. They puree their food to get through their [doughnut-shaped] brain," he said. "Their skin has evolved this amazing capacity to do black, white, colour and reflection — turning on and off sections of their skin." Cuttlefish's oesophagus goes through the middle of their brain, so they need to puree their food so it fits through. ( Getty Images: Nigel Marsh ) This ability to change colour, shape and even texture can be used to camouflage and fight, as well as create complex mating displays. They've been called the "rock stars of the sea", as they will mate multiple times during just one season, and die after just a year or two, once the next generation is born. Are these shape-shifters your favourite underrated animal? If so, vote below! Sharpshooter: Velvet worm The velvet worm may look cute, but it is a ferocious hunter. ( ABC ) They shoot glue, puree their enemies, and are about the size of a leaf. With 140 species of southern velvet worms ( Peripatopsidae ), and most of those in Australia, these tiny creatures are our homegrown invertebrate. They normally live in rainforests, under logs or leaf litter, and they like to keep it damp and dark. The starry night velvet worm is the only species of velvet worm that hunts and feeds in groups. ( Supplied: Latty lab ) Interestingly, velvet worms are not worms, caterpillars or even an insect — they're actually most closely related to the tardigrade. According to Tanya Latty, an entomologist from the University of Sydney, these cute little critters are "voracious, stone-cold predators". They shoot a sticky glue-like substance at their prey — normally insects and other invertebrates — and when their target is all tied up, velvet worms produce a substance that can digest the prey. "It turns into a kind of squishy milkshake on the inside," Dr Latty said. "Then they slurp out all that goodness." Eek! If this adorable little predator is your favourite underrated animal, don't forget to vote below. Mysterious: Marsupial mole The marsupial mole is one of 10 underrated native animals people can vote for this National Science Week. ( ABC ) What has no eyes, no external ears, and is barely ever seen? It's no riddle — it's the marsupial mole! These tiny, golden creatures like to burrow through the sands in central Australia, and it's not easy to catch them. OK, maybe even that is an understatement. According to Natalie Warburton, a zoologist at Murdoch University, the two species of marsupial mole ( Notoryctes ) are almost impossible to find. "They're the most cryptic and enigmatic of any of the living species of marsupial," she said. "Nobody ever sees them." The marsupial mole is functionally blind, and has no pupils. ( Supplied: Ian Harris ) They are extremely well adapted to a life lived burrowing through the sand. They have a kangaroo-like tail, a pouch that faces backwards and ginormous claws. But they're also functionally blind, and only have pin holes for ears, as these are no longer required for them to find a mate or a meal — which is mostly insects. If the mysterious marsupial mole has your heart for the best underrated animal, don't forget to vote below! Musician: Palm cockatoo The palm cockatoo is one of 10 underrated native animals people can vote for this National Science Week. ( ABC ) It's a bold claim, but the palm cockatoo might be the most punk-rock bird of all. The black and scarlet parrot lives in the rainforests and woodlands of Far North Queensland, as well as the Aru Islands and New Guinea. Palm cockatoos breed in old-growth tree hollows, and they only lay eggs every second year. ( Supplied: Christina Zdenek ) With a wingspan of 1.5 metres, the palm cockatoo (Probosciger aterrimus) is an impressive bird, with a black mohawk and a powerful beak, according to Christina Zdenek, an ecologist for People for Wildlife. "When you're face-to-face with a palm cockatoo and you see the size of that bill and that lower bill, it's like a shovel," she said. "And you can see them snip through sticks as big as my thumb." The palm cockatoo also has a completely unique ability to drum, which the males do with a large stick held in their feet. If drummers are your thing, vote for the palm cockatoo as the most underrated animal below. Kinship: Great desert skink The desert skink is one of 10 underrated native animals people can vote for this National Science Week. ( ABC ) This little orange lizard lives with its mum and poos outside. What's not to love? The great desert skink ( Liopholis kintorei) can be found in the deserts of the Northern Territory, South Australia, and Western Australia. It lives in large burrows with its family, which is extremely unusual for reptiles. The great desert skink is known by many different Indigenous names, including tjakura, mulyamiji and warrarna. ( Supplied: Kerenza Sunfly ) Christine Ellis Michaels, a Warlpiri ranger and master tracker, says she can tell who is living in the burrow from their outdoor toilet. "When we see the scat outside, we know who is living in the burrow — mum, dad and the little ones," she said. The great desert skink is a species of cultural significance for many Aboriginal groups. The lizards thrive in areas where Indigenous rangers conduct traditional burning or cat management. If this magnificent skink is your underrated animal of choice, don't forget to vote below. Secretive: Short-finned eel The eel is one of 10 underrated native animals people can vote for this National Science Week. ( ABC ) A true underdog, the short-finned eel might not be your favourite animal. But we're here to show you why Anguilla australis is actually a strong contender for your vote. First, they have a surprisingly long life, according to Wayne Koster from the Arthur Rylah Institute for Environmental Research. "Some of these eels live for 10, 20, even 30 years," he said. The short-finned eel dives almost a kilometre below the surface of the ocean each day on its mating migration. ( Supplied: Kim Birnie-Gauvin ) You can find them throughout the waterways of South Australia, Tasmania, Victoria and New South Wales, as well as some places in New Zealand and the South Pacific. They can travel over land, climb walls and take down large prey. And that's all before they make it out into the ocean for their epic migration to mate. The location is so remote, scientists still don't know what happens while they're there. But then, as new baby eels, they need to swim thousands of kilometres back to the Australian mainland — and only one in every 10,000 hatchings make it. While they might look a bit fishy, the short-finned eel may be your favourite underrated animal. If so, don't forget to vote below. Burrower: Turtle frog The turtle frog is one of 10 underrated native animals people can vote for this National Science Week. ( ABC ) This tiny frog is pasty white, with big arms, a teeny head, and is — somehow — still absolutely adorable. The turtle frog ( Myobatrachus gouldii ) can be found in a small southern part of Western Australia near Geraldton. It mostly lives underground, and is one of only a few species of frog that burrows forwards instead of backwards. Turtle frogs lay eggs, but their young skip the tadpole stage, and hatch directly into tiny frogs. ( Supplied: Ross McGibbon Photography ) As a result, the frog has particularly large arms, and the appearance — according to Paul Doughty, the curator of herpetology at the Western Australian Museum — of Dwayne "The Rock" Johnson. "They have these giant, super-buff arms," he said. "The Latin name for the genus Myobatrachus literally translated means 'muscle frog.'" Turtle frogs can also skip the tadpole stage, and spend months with their significant other underground. If you think the muscle frog is the most underrated animal, let us know by voting below! Familial: Dugong The dugong is one of 10 underrated native animals people can vote for this National Science Week. ( ABC ) They live off seagrass, and sound like a teeny bird, but can weigh in at more than 400 kilograms and will cut you with their tusks. The dugong ( Dugong dugon ) is no animal to mess with. But while they were once confused for mermaids, dugongs do have many similarities to humans, according to Rachel Groom, a marine researcher at Charles Darwin University. "They can live up to 70 years of age, and they don't have calves every year. They don't mature till they're 13 to 15 years of age," she said. Dugongs use fine hairs on their nose to detect sea grass and dig it up. ( Getty Images: Andrea Izzotti ) There's also a real familial connection, which groups like the li-Anthawirriyarra Sea Ranger Unit from the south-western Gulf of Carpentaria are well aware of. "What we've learned from working with Yanyuwa people is that they have identified 16 different types of dugong," Dr Groom said. "They have the old female dugong, the young female dugong, they've got names for calves, they've got names for the lead dugong. They've got the name for the lead whistling dugong, and the one that should never be touched." Are these giant sea cows your favourite underrated animal? If so, give them a vote below. Adorable: Rakali The rakali is one of 10 underrated native animals people can vote for this National Science Week. ( ABC ) Sure, it's a rat, but it's Australia's own cheeky little water rat! The rakali ( Hydromys chrysogaster ) is Australia's largest rodent, and can be found in the waterways all around Australia. According to Emmalie Sanders, an ecologist at Charles Sturt University, rakali are extremely resilient to water quality changes, so they can be found almost anywhere. The rakali is one of only a few animals that has learnt to flip cane toads onto their back so they can eat them safely. ( Flickr: Rakali, audiodam, CC BY-NC-SA 2.0 ) "If you have a local billabong or lagoon, a wetland area, they're most likely there. Even in coastal areas or estuaries," she said. "A lot of people liken them to an otter. So they fill that niche here." They're also quite wily, and are able to slip out of the collars scientists use to study them, and have been been caught eating underwater microphones just to get some privacy. If the clever rakali is your favourite underrated animal, make sure you give it a vote below! Can't decide? Keep an eye out for videos highlighting each creature's best features rolling out over the next two weeks on Facebook and Instagram, and special mini episodes of What the Duck?! with Dr Ann Jones on the ABC Listen App. Voting closes at 11.30AM AEST on Friday, August 15 and then we'll unveil the winner!

ABC News
4 hours ago
- ABC News
Amy Bloom, Ben Markovits and Barbara Truelove on love, basketball and monsters
Amy Bloom on her latest novel I'll Be Right Here about an unconventional chosen family, Ben Markovits goes on the road with his Booker Prize longlisted novel The Rest of Our Lives and Barbara Truelove's bonkers book about Dracula in space, Of Monsters and Mainframes. Amy Bloom is the American author of ten books (including White Houses) and her new historical novel, I'll Be Right Here, begins in wartime Paris and follows an unconventional, chosen family into the 21st century. The famous French author Collette has a cameo role too. Amy Bloom also shares the two things that matter to her most and why she writes about love in all its forms. Of Monsters and Mainframes is the debut novel of the Australian author and game designer Barbara Truelove. It's a genre mash of science fiction and pulp horror and is largely narrated by a sentient spaceship. The Rest of Our Lives is the 12th novel by British-American writer Benjamin Markovits and has recently been longlisted for the Booker Prize. It follows Tom, who's in a middle aged rut, as he sets out on a road trip across America and visits people from his past. Ben also talks about his failed career as a professional basketball player, the parallels between basketball and writing, and how a health crisis enriched the writing of this latest book.