logo
Orcas sharing prey with humans may be trying to learn ‘who we are,' researcher says

Orcas sharing prey with humans may be trying to learn ‘who we are,' researcher says

CTV News5 days ago
An orca offers a diver a piece of Eagle Ray liver off New Zealand in the Western South Pacific as shown in this handout image provided by Orca Research Trust. THE CANADIAN PRESS/Handout — Orca Research Trust (Mandatory Credit)
Jared Towers was in his research vessel on two separate occasions watching killer whales off the coast of Vancouver Island when the orcas dropped their prey directly in front of him and his colleagues.
The encounters he describes as 'rare' and awe-inspiring have led to a new study published in the peer-reviewed Journal of Comparative Psychology, detailing researchers' experiences with killer whales apparently sharing their food with humans.
'We have a long history of interacting with other animals, trying to feed them and gauging their responses. But it's very rare for any wild predator to do the same to us,' says Towers, who is the executive director of the research group Bay Cetology.
'This is really the first report of any kind in the literature documenting these cases for killer whales.'
Towers says he and his colleagues were cruising along when an orca appeared, setting off the second encounter in 2018. They stopped and watched it swim around before it reappeared and released a freshly killed seal next to the boat.
'She could have dropped it off the stern or the bow, but she dropped it right in the middle of the vessel, right next to us,' says Towers.
'We just sat there watching this thing sink down into the water for about 10 or 15 seconds until she did a bit of a circle and came back and picked it up.'
It followed an encounter in 2015 that involved an orca opening its mouth and releasing a dead ancient murrelet, a kind of seabird, directly beside Towers' boat. He says that orca also left its prey floating for a few moments before taking it again.
'It left us a bit awestruck,' says Towers, adding the two cases stand out among the thousands of encounters with killer whales he's had around the world.
'I started to think about these animals a bit differently at that point.'
Towers and his colleagues began an investigation that led to the study published on Monday, which examines 34 instances in which killer whales around the world appeared to offer their prey to humans.
The researchers wanted to ensure the study only examined cases where whales were 'going out of their way to engage with people rather than vice versa,' says Towers, who is based in Alert Bay, B.C., off northwestern Vancouver Island.
In order to be included in the study, the whales had to approach humans directly. Researchers considered cases where people had not approached the whales at a distance closer than 50 metres in the five minutes prior to the interaction taking place.
In all but one of the situations, the study says the whales were observed waiting for people to respond before either recovering or abandoning their prey.
'These weren't mistakes. They weren't like the killer whales accidentally dropped the food. They wanted to see how people responded,' Towers says.
The study does not rule out any selfish motivations behind the behaviour. But Towers says he feels the apparent prey sharing is 'altruistic' and 'pro-social.'
Sharing food among relatives and other orcas is foundational for the whales, and in attempting to provision humans with prey, he says it could be an example of the whales practicing a cultural behaviour or exploring humans' capacity to respond.
'I think these cases may really be pro-social representations of conscious learning where these whales are going out of their way to actually try and understand ... who we are and how we might interact with them in their environment,' Towers says.
Given the advanced cognitive abilities and the social, co-operative nature of killer whales as a species, the study says the researchers 'assume that any or all these explanations for, and outcomes of, such behaviour are possible.'
The whales in the study were transient orcas off the coasts of B.C. and Alaska, the Eastern Tropical Pacific population off the coast of California, along with killer whales off the coasts of New Zealand, central Argentina and Norway. All of the 'offering' events took place between 2004 and 2024, the study says.
Orcas commonly use prey to engage in play, and the study acknowledges that 38 per cent of the prey-sharing cases it examined appeared to incorporate play. The whales may have been using their prey to instigate play with humans, it says.
But for several reasons, the study says the researchers do not believe play was the driving factor behind the apparent offerings.
Play often occurs after whales have met their nutritional needs, but in the cases of prey sharing with humans, the offerings were whole in about half of the encounters.
The orcas in the study mostly recovered the prey after it wasn't accepted by humans and often went on to share it with other whales. In most cases, the interactions did not last longer than 30 seconds. By contrast, the study says the whales typically engage in play more continuously.
The study concludes the whales possess the capacity and motivation to share food for multiple reasons that could include intellectual or emotional benefits.
'Offering items to humans could simultaneously include opportunities for killer whales to practice learned cultural behaviour, explore, or play and in so doing learn about, manipulate, or develop relationships with us,' it says.
Towers says he hopes the study provides an opportunity for people to look at killer whales in a different light, sparking curiosity about their capacity to think 'and perhaps even have some convergent evolution of intellect with us.'
The researchers strongly discourage people from accepting any prey offered by orcas, due to the potential for both species to harm one another, Towers adds.
This report by Brenna Owen, The Canadian Press was first published June 30, 2025.
Orange background

Try Our AI Features

Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:

Comments

No comments yet...

Related Articles

Orcas might be trying to learn 'who we are' when they share prey with humans, study suggests
Orcas might be trying to learn 'who we are' when they share prey with humans, study suggests

CBC

time4 days ago

  • CBC

Orcas might be trying to learn 'who we are' when they share prey with humans, study suggests

Jared Towers was in his research vessel on two separate occasions watching killer whales off the coast of Vancouver Island when the orcas dropped their prey directly in front of him and his colleagues. The encounters he describes as "rare" and awe-inspiring have led to a new study published in the peer-reviewed Journal of Comparative Psychology, detailing researchers' experiences with killer whales apparently sharing their food with humans. "We have a long history of interacting with other animals, trying to feed them and gauging their responses. But it's very rare for any wild predator to do the same to us," said Towers, who is the executive director of the research group Bay Cetology. "This is really the first report of any kind in the literature documenting these cases for killer whales." Towers says he and his colleagues were cruising along when an orca appeared, setting off the second encounter in 2018. They stopped and watched it swim around before it reappeared and released a freshly killed seal next to the boat. "She could have dropped it off the stern or the bow, but she dropped it right in the middle of the vessel, right next to us," Towers said. "We just sat there watching this thing sink down into the water for about 10 or 15 seconds until she did a bit of a circle and came back and picked it up." It followed an encounter in 2015 that involved an orca opening its mouth and releasing a dead ancient murrelet, a kind of seabird, directly beside Towers's boat. He says that orca also left its prey floating for a few moments before taking it again. "It left us a bit awestruck," said Towers, adding the two cases stand out among the thousands of encounters with killer whales he's had around the world. "I started to think about these animals a bit differently at that point." Towers and his colleagues began an investigation that led to the study published on Monday, which examines 34 instances in which killer whales around the world appeared to offer their prey to humans. The researchers wanted to ensure the study only examined cases where whales were "going out of their way to engage with people rather than vice versa," said Towers, who is based in Alert Bay, B.C., off northeastern Vancouver Island. In order to be included in the study, the whales had to approach humans directly. Researchers considered cases where people had not approached the whales at a distance closer than 50 metres in the five minutes prior to the interaction taking place. In all but one of the situations, the study says the whales were observed waiting for people to respond before either recovering or abandoning their prey. "These weren't mistakes. They weren't like the killer whales accidentally dropped the food. They wanted to see how people responded," Towers said. The study does not rule out any selfish motivations behind the behaviour. But Towers says he feels the apparent prey sharing is "altruistic" and "pro-social." Sharing food among relatives and other orcas is foundational for the whales, and in attempting to provision humans with prey, he says it could be an example of the whales practicing a cultural behaviour or exploring humans' capacity to respond. WATCH | Orca whales spotted off Cape Breton in 'once in a lifetime' experience: Orca whales spotted off Cape Breton in 'once in a lifetime' experience 26 days ago Duration 0:52 "I think these cases may really be pro-social representations of conscious learning where these whales are going out of their way to actually try and understand ... who we are and how we might interact with them in their environment," Towers said. Given the advanced cognitive abilities and the social, co-operative nature of killer whales as a species, the study says the researchers "assume that any or all these explanations for, and outcomes of, such behaviour are possible." The whales in the study were transient orcas off the coasts of B.C. and Alaska, the Eastern Tropical Pacific population off the coast of California, along with killer whales around New Zealand, central Argentina and Norway. All of the "offering" events took place between 2004 and 2024, the study says. Orcas commonly use prey to engage in play, and the study acknowledges that 38 per cent of the prey-sharing cases it examined appeared to incorporate play. The whales may have been using their prey to instigate play with humans, it says. But for several reasons, the study says the researchers do not believe play was the driving factor behind the apparent offerings. WATCH | The moment an orca 'moonwalked' off the coast of West Vancouver: #TheMoment an orca 'moonwalked' off the coast of West Vancouver 22 days ago Duration 1:18 Allison MacGillivary recounts the moment she and her family spotted a killer whale swimming backwards off the shoreline in West Vancouver. Play often occurs after whales have met their nutritional needs, but in the cases of prey sharing with humans, the offerings were whole in about half of the encounters. The orcas in the study mostly recovered the prey after it wasn't accepted by humans and often went on to share it with other whales. In most cases, the interactions did not last longer than 30 seconds. By contrast, the study says the whales typically engage in play more continuously. The study concludes the whales possess the capacity and motivation to share food for multiple reasons that could include intellectual or emotional benefits. "Offering items to humans could simultaneously include opportunities for killer whales to practice learned cultural behaviour, explore or play and in so doing learn about, manipulate or develop relationships with us," it said. Towers says he hopes the study provides an opportunity for people to look at killer whales in a different light, sparking curiosity about their capacity to think "and perhaps even have some convergent evolution of intellect with us." The researchers strongly discourage people from accepting any prey offered by orcas, due to the potential for both species to harm one another, Towers added.

Orcas sharing prey with humans may be trying to learn ‘who we are,' researcher says
Orcas sharing prey with humans may be trying to learn ‘who we are,' researcher says

CTV News

time5 days ago

  • CTV News

Orcas sharing prey with humans may be trying to learn ‘who we are,' researcher says

An orca offers a diver a piece of Eagle Ray liver off New Zealand in the Western South Pacific as shown in this handout image provided by Orca Research Trust. THE CANADIAN PRESS/Handout — Orca Research Trust (Mandatory Credit) Jared Towers was in his research vessel on two separate occasions watching killer whales off the coast of Vancouver Island when the orcas dropped their prey directly in front of him and his colleagues. The encounters he describes as 'rare' and awe-inspiring have led to a new study published in the peer-reviewed Journal of Comparative Psychology, detailing researchers' experiences with killer whales apparently sharing their food with humans. 'We have a long history of interacting with other animals, trying to feed them and gauging their responses. But it's very rare for any wild predator to do the same to us,' says Towers, who is the executive director of the research group Bay Cetology. 'This is really the first report of any kind in the literature documenting these cases for killer whales.' Towers says he and his colleagues were cruising along when an orca appeared, setting off the second encounter in 2018. They stopped and watched it swim around before it reappeared and released a freshly killed seal next to the boat. 'She could have dropped it off the stern or the bow, but she dropped it right in the middle of the vessel, right next to us,' says Towers. 'We just sat there watching this thing sink down into the water for about 10 or 15 seconds until she did a bit of a circle and came back and picked it up.' It followed an encounter in 2015 that involved an orca opening its mouth and releasing a dead ancient murrelet, a kind of seabird, directly beside Towers' boat. He says that orca also left its prey floating for a few moments before taking it again. 'It left us a bit awestruck,' says Towers, adding the two cases stand out among the thousands of encounters with killer whales he's had around the world. 'I started to think about these animals a bit differently at that point.' Towers and his colleagues began an investigation that led to the study published on Monday, which examines 34 instances in which killer whales around the world appeared to offer their prey to humans. The researchers wanted to ensure the study only examined cases where whales were 'going out of their way to engage with people rather than vice versa,' says Towers, who is based in Alert Bay, B.C., off northwestern Vancouver Island. In order to be included in the study, the whales had to approach humans directly. Researchers considered cases where people had not approached the whales at a distance closer than 50 metres in the five minutes prior to the interaction taking place. In all but one of the situations, the study says the whales were observed waiting for people to respond before either recovering or abandoning their prey. 'These weren't mistakes. They weren't like the killer whales accidentally dropped the food. They wanted to see how people responded,' Towers says. The study does not rule out any selfish motivations behind the behaviour. But Towers says he feels the apparent prey sharing is 'altruistic' and 'pro-social.' Sharing food among relatives and other orcas is foundational for the whales, and in attempting to provision humans with prey, he says it could be an example of the whales practicing a cultural behaviour or exploring humans' capacity to respond. 'I think these cases may really be pro-social representations of conscious learning where these whales are going out of their way to actually try and understand ... who we are and how we might interact with them in their environment,' Towers says. Given the advanced cognitive abilities and the social, co-operative nature of killer whales as a species, the study says the researchers 'assume that any or all these explanations for, and outcomes of, such behaviour are possible.' The whales in the study were transient orcas off the coasts of B.C. and Alaska, the Eastern Tropical Pacific population off the coast of California, along with killer whales off the coasts of New Zealand, central Argentina and Norway. All of the 'offering' events took place between 2004 and 2024, the study says. Orcas commonly use prey to engage in play, and the study acknowledges that 38 per cent of the prey-sharing cases it examined appeared to incorporate play. The whales may have been using their prey to instigate play with humans, it says. But for several reasons, the study says the researchers do not believe play was the driving factor behind the apparent offerings. Play often occurs after whales have met their nutritional needs, but in the cases of prey sharing with humans, the offerings were whole in about half of the encounters. The orcas in the study mostly recovered the prey after it wasn't accepted by humans and often went on to share it with other whales. In most cases, the interactions did not last longer than 30 seconds. By contrast, the study says the whales typically engage in play more continuously. The study concludes the whales possess the capacity and motivation to share food for multiple reasons that could include intellectual or emotional benefits. 'Offering items to humans could simultaneously include opportunities for killer whales to practice learned cultural behaviour, explore, or play and in so doing learn about, manipulate, or develop relationships with us,' it says. Towers says he hopes the study provides an opportunity for people to look at killer whales in a different light, sparking curiosity about their capacity to think 'and perhaps even have some convergent evolution of intellect with us.' The researchers strongly discourage people from accepting any prey offered by orcas, due to the potential for both species to harm one another, Towers adds. This report by Brenna Owen, The Canadian Press was first published June 30, 2025.

Killer whales use seaweed as tools to groom each other
Killer whales use seaweed as tools to groom each other

CTV News

time24-06-2025

  • CTV News

Killer whales use seaweed as tools to groom each other

A baby orca named L125, swims with its mother, L86, in this undated handout photo. THE CANADIAN PRESS/HO, Dave Ellifrit, Center for Whale Research Killer whales are known for exceptional intelligence, displaying complex social structures and sophisticated communication. New research provides fresh evidence for this, documenting how these marine mammals use stalks of seaweed as tools to groom each other - as in, 'You scratch my back and I'll scratch yours.' Using drones to observe a population of killer whales in the Salish Sea, part of the Pacific Ocean between Washington state and British Columbia, researchers noticed that these predators engaged in a behaviour they named 'allokelping,' one of the few known examples of tool use by marine mammals. The killer whales find large stalks of a type of seaweed called bull kelp, either attached to the seabed or floating at the surface. They then bite off the end of the stalk, position it between themselves and another killer whale and roll the kelp between their bodies. The researchers hypothesize that the behavior promotes skin health while strengthening social bonds. Other populations of killer whales have been observed rubbing their bodies on smooth stone beaches, possibly to remove dead skin. 'Most examples of tool use in animals involve solving ecological problems, such as gaining access to food. For example, chimpanzees use sticks to fish for termites. What's remarkable about this discovery is that the tool - the kelp - is used not to obtain food but to facilitate social interaction,' said marine biologist Darren Croft of the University of Exeter in England, co-author of the study published this week in the journal Current Biology. 'This kind of socially motivated tool use is extremely rare in non-human animals and has previously only been observed in a small number of primates, usually in captivity,' added Croft, executive director of the Center for Whale Research, a scientific organization based in Washington state that has studied this population of killer whales since the 1970s. The researchers documented the behavior among both male and female killer whales of all ages. It likely plays an important role in their social lives, Croft said. There are some other examples of tool use among marine mammals. Sea otters use rocks and other hard objects to crack open shells to get at the meat inside. And certain dolphins use marine sponges to protect their snouts and stir up the seabed while foraging. The researchers said the behaviour by the killer whales goes one step further because they modify an object for use as a tool. 'While this is not the first documented case of cetacean or marine mammal tool use, it is - as far as we know - the first case of cetacean tool manufacturing with tool use. The whales are not just finding perfect lengths of kelp in the environment, but rather actively modifying larger intact stalks of kelp to create the pieces they are using for allokelping,' said study co-author Rachel John, a University of Exeter graduate student studying killer whale behaviour. 'Another key part of what makes this behaviour so unique is the fact that they are manipulating the kelp cooperatively with a partner without the use of hands or any hand-like appendages. They use their mouth to position the kelp initially, but after that they only use the momentum and pressure of the core of their bodies to maintain contact with each other and the kelp between them,' John added. The behaviour is known only among this killer whale population. 'We found that individuals with more visible peeling skin were more likely to engage in allokelping, suggesting that the behavior may serve a skin-care function. Brown seaweeds like kelp are known to have antibacterial properties, so it's plausible that rubbing with kelp helps improve skin health,' Croft said. 'Second, we think this behavior likely plays a role in maintaining social bonds. Physical contact is known to be important for social cohesion in many species, including humans. Just as we might hug a friend we haven't seen in a while, it's possible that allokelping serves to reinforce social relationships among whales,' Croft added. This kelp grows in cold and nutrient-rich coastal and intertidal waters and thrives off the western coast of North America, which includes the home range of these killer whales. This population is at grave risk of extinction, Croft said, with only 73 individuals counted in the latest census. They are highly specialized salmon hunters, particularly reliant on Chinook salmon. As salmon populations have declined, in part due to dam-building on spawning rivers, the whales have struggled to find enough food. 'In short, they are starving,' Croft said. By Will Dunham (Reporting by Will Dunham in Washington, Editing by Rosalba O'Brien)

DOWNLOAD THE APP

Get Started Now: Download the App

Ready to dive into a world of global content with local flavor? Download Daily8 app today from your preferred app store and start exploring.
app-storeplay-store