logo
Wild Orcas Sometimes Offer To Share Their Lunch With Humans

Wild Orcas Sometimes Offer To Share Their Lunch With Humans

Forbes3 days ago
They amuse us by wearing salmon hats, enrage us by sinking our expensive yachts, and now they have been documented sharing their meals with us – why?
Food sharing amongst orcas can also be extended to include food sharing with humans. (Credit: Jared ... More Towers with kind permission.)
A recent report reveals that wild orcas, Orcinus orca, also known as killer whales, sometimes offer food to humans, according to the report's lead author, Jared Towers, executive director of Bay Cetology, a whale research organization based in British Columbia.
Orcas are the oceans' apex predators – the top predators in the seas – and they have correspondingly large brains that are second only to humans in relation to their body size. So clearly, they are capable of advanced communication skills (even including mimicking human speech; more here), cognition and emotional intelligence.
Furthermore, prey sharing is common in orca culture, so maybe sharing their lunch with humans is not really so strange after all. In fact, throughout the world, orcas of every age have presented prey to humans, and just about everything was on the menu: from sea otters, harbor seals, and gray whales, to green turtles, eagle rays, starfish, jellyfish, and even common murres, just to name a few species.
F I G U R E 1 : Spatial and Temporal Overview of Provisioning Attempts Note. (A) The locations of ... More each offering. (B) The timeline of events by geographic region. ENA = Eastern North Atlantic; ENP = Eastern North Pacific; ETP = Eastern Tropical Pacific; WSA = Western South Atlantic; WSP = Western South Pacific.
Dr Towers, along with a team of researchers from Aotearoa New Zealand and Mexico investigated 34 interactions that occurred during the past 20 years when wild orcas attempted to share prey with people in various locations of the Pacific and Atlantic oceans from California to Norway to Patagonia (Figure 1).
F I G U R E 2 : Locations of People and Status of Prey During Provisioning Attempts Note. (A) Where ... More people were located when offerings were made in each geographic region. (B) The status and condition of items being offered by geographic region. ENP = Eastern North Pacific; ETP = Eastern Tropical Pacific; WSP = Western South Pacific; WSA = Western South Atlantic; ENA = Eastern North Atlantic.
For this study, Dr Towers and collaborators only included incidents that met particular criteria: the whales had to have approached the people on their own volition rather than the people closely approaching the whales, and dropped the prey item in front of them.
It's interesting to point out that in all but one of the cases, the orcas waited to see what would happen after they made the offering, and in seven cases, the orcas tried more than once to offer the prey item after the people initially refused it. On 11 of the occasions, people were in the water when the orcas approached them, in 21 cases, they were on boats, and in two cases they were on the shore (Figure 2). Some of the interactions were captured on video and in photos; others were described in interviews with the researchers.
A pair of wild Orcas breaching in Alaskan coastal waters. (Via PickPik / CC0, Public domain.)
Clearly, the orcas have a reason for sharing food with humans, but what is it?
'Orcas often share food with each other – it's a prosocial activity and a way that they build relationships with each other,' Dr Towers explained. 'That they also share with humans may show their interest in relating to us as well.'
According to Dr Towers and collaborators, this altruistic behavior makes sense because orcas are intelligent and highly social animals that use food sharing as a way to build relationships with kin as well as unrelated individuals. Further, because orcas often hunt prey much larger than themselves, they may sometimes have food to spare.
'I suspect that individual killer whales from several populations around the world having been documented offering people prey reflects how foundational prey sharing and any associated reciprocity is to the evolution of the species (like it is in humans),' Dr Towers told me in email. 'This said, some populations do it more than others, which is not surprising given their differing levels of dietary specialization, resource availability, etc.'
Orca dorsal fin with West Point lighthouse in background. (Credit: Discovery Park Staff, Seattle ... More Parks and Recreation / CC BY 4.0)
As a native of Seattle, I was disappointed to learn that the local wild orca family pods (sometimes known as 'resident orcas' to distinguish them from the more widely roaming 'transient orcas') have never been known to share their lunches with humans – although they are famous for wearing 'salmon hats' on their heads.
'Some readers may wonder if resident orcas have ever been documented offering people food and if not, why. The answer is no, they have not, despite decades of research on them,' Dr Towers told me in email. 'This may be because being strict dietary specialists they have less to gain through exploratory behaviour to acquire general knowledge in their environment. Furthermore, there may be times when they can not afford to share their food because their prey resources are often limited.'
Family of orcas swimming near an expensive yacht that may be fleeing from them
That said, it is interesting to note that another group of orcas – naughty, mischievous orcas – living along the Iberian Peninsula and coastal Spain that seem to delight in sinking rich people's yachts will sometimes offer people chunks of tuna.
How should people react if an orca tries to share food with them? Although the behavior is rare, Dr Towers says that you should document the interaction either with photographs or film, without touching the offering, and share it with the behavioral scientists at Bay Centology so we can learn more about this unusual behavior.
What do these examples of wild orcas offering to share their lunches with us tell us about them?
'I think in some cases these whales are very curious and this is just highlighting exactly how curious they can become,' Dr Towers replied.
'[K]iller whales are very complex and evolved animals,' Dr Towers added, 'and that we should consider this when it comes to co-existing with them as top apex predators of different biomes on this planet.'
Source:
Jared R. Towers , Ingrid N. Visser, and Vanessa Prigollini (2025). Testing the Waters: Attempts by Wild Killer Whales (Orcinus orca) to Provision People (Homo sapiens), Journal of Comparative Psychology | doi:10.1037/com0000422
© Copyright by GrrlScientist | hosted by Forbes | LinkTr.ee
Socials: Bluesky | CounterSocial | LinkedIn | Mastodon Science | MeWe | Spoutible | SubStack | Threads | Tumblr | Twitter
Orange background

Try Our AI Features

Explore what Daily8 AI can do for you:

Comments

No comments yet...

Related Articles

This Weird Pyramid Always Lands on the Same Face, Confirming 40-Year-Old Theory
This Weird Pyramid Always Lands on the Same Face, Confirming 40-Year-Old Theory

Gizmodo

time4 hours ago

  • Gizmodo

This Weird Pyramid Always Lands on the Same Face, Confirming 40-Year-Old Theory

'Bille' is the first-ever monostable tetrahedron, or a pyramid-like shape with four triangular faces that has one stable resting position. What this means is that Bille, no matter how you throw it and how it lands, will flip back on exactly the same side every single time. In a recent preprint submitted to arXiv, mathematicians revealed the first physical model of Bille, closing a decades-old theory proposed by the renowned British mathematician John Conway. Made of lightweight carbon fiber and dense tungsten carbide, Bille represents an array of ridiculously sophisticated engineering decisions—making this as much a technological achievement as a mathematical one. It's no surprise, therefore, that its self-righting property additionally hints at some exciting applications for the spaceflight industry—which notably experienced two recent landing mishaps with toppled-over lunar landers. In his initial conjecture, Conway surmised that a tetrahedron with unevenly distributed weight across its sides would always flip to the same side, although a few years later Conway himself rejected the idea. Some mathematicians still thought there could be something to it, however, namely study co-author Robert Dawson, who almost succeeded in proving Conway right in the 1980s using lead foil and sticks of bamboo. 'But my recollection was that this only almost worked because of angular momentum,' Dawson, now a mathematician at Saint Mary's University in Canada, told Gizmodo. 'In the way that if a car comes across a bump in the road and it's already moving, it'll get over it thanks to angular momentum. But it might have a hard time starting up against that bump.' Ideally, the monostable tetrahedron shouldn't need another push to flop back on the 'base' side. For a while, it seemed like Conway's theory would end up in a box of really-cool-but-unlikely math ideas—until about three years ago, when mathematician Gábor Domokos and his student, Gergő Almádi at the Budapest University of Technology and Economics, reached out to Dawson. Domokos, a long-time expert on tricky balancing problems in geometry, had already discovered the gömböc, a roundish object that balances only on two points like a roly-poly toy. While an impressive discovery, the gömböc, with its mostly round, multi-sided design, features relatively easy conditions for self-balancing, Domoko told Gizmodo. The fewer sides a figure has and the smaller the angles are on each side, the harder it is to make that figure monostable, he said. Picture the common six-sided die. 'If it is a fair die, it will land on each face with equal probability,' Domoko explained. Even if someone cheats and modifies the die by putting some extra weight on a couple of surfaces, the probability will shift slightly, but it should still be possible for the die to stand on all its faces. In that sense, the tetrahedron, with its pointy corners and tiny acute angles across its four sides, makes it the 'most difficult problem, the highest category' of shapes in terms of monostability—barring some kind of engineering miracle. Which really happened. After deriving a theoretical model to calculate Bille's dimensions, Almádi, an architecture student, spearheaded the quest to build a structure that, somehow, had one side made from a 'really heavy material, the lighter parts almost air, and an almost empty skeleton,' Domokos said. The team settled on carbon tubes for the skeleton and, for the base, dense tungsten carbide—a metal alloy twice as heavy as steel. Even after all that, an issue remained: For some reason, Bille kept landing on two different sides, not the one intended side. 'Then we looked at it, and there was a very small glob of glue which was sticking to one end!' Domoko exclaimed. Despite the chief engineer's assurances that it made no difference, Domoko insisted on removing the tiny blob of glue—the density and shape of which were also calculated with ridiculous precision. And—voilà. Bille made mathematical history. That said, the engineers played a huge role in making this possible, Domokos clarified. 'They were all part of the creation process—the geometry, engineering, and technological design. They all needed to click. If you take out any of these, it doesn't work.' To make sure Bille wasn't just a one-time dud, Domokos' team succeeded in making a second model—though this probably isn't something one could easily make at home. 'We wish good luck to anyone doing it,' Domokos joked. 'But somebody doing it now has a huge advantage compared to us, because we didn't know whether it would work.' Domokos is particularly excited to see what might become of Bille further down the line. One reason Domokos didn't want to stop at merely modeling Bille was because of gömböc, he explained. Like many aesthetically pleasing mathematical breakthroughs, gömböc got a lot of love from artistic communities and natural scientists drawing parallels between turtle shells and gömböc—which Domokos more or less expected. What he didn't expect was that Novo Nordisk, in collaboration with MIT and Harvard, would take interest in gömböc's design principles for an insulin capsule that self-rights itself once inside a stomach, eliminating the need for needle injections. 'And it sounded so outlandish—like science fiction,' Domokos said. 'Gömböc taught me that physical objects are crucial—there are many bright people out there who are not mathematically minded, but they can look at something and it will reflect in their minds many other things.' Still, it'll probably be a while—if ever—before Bille ends up in the blueprint for the latest lunar lander, which Domokos knows will be extremely challenging. 'When you develop something, you have to wait and technological innovation will catch up. Sometimes it takes 100 years, sometimes it takes 10 years. Mathematics is always a little bit ahead.'

Scientists make disturbing discovery at one of most remote locations on Earth: 'Increased levels of the stress hormone'
Scientists make disturbing discovery at one of most remote locations on Earth: 'Increased levels of the stress hormone'

Yahoo

time11 hours ago

  • Yahoo

Scientists make disturbing discovery at one of most remote locations on Earth: 'Increased levels of the stress hormone'

The noise pollution caused by human intervention in Antarctica could be negatively affecting the well-being of local wildlife, a study has revealed. Researchers at Uruguay's University of the Republic and Spain's Pompeu Fabra University explored whether a power generator could be heard from 2 kilometers away in an Antarctic Specially Protected Area called Ardley Island. The location is an important breeding ground for several seabird species, and marine mammals frequent the area to molt and feed. It is also, however, near the Fildes Peninsula, which is one of the most densely populated places in Antarctica, with bases set up by multiple countries. Through the use of recording devices that monitored sound for five minutes every hour, the team concluded that the generator in question was heard from the protected area 2 km away. Research has shown that human noise can be a significant stressor on wild animal populations, impacting communication and social interactions among species. As noise encroaches on an animal's ecosystem, it can have concerning consequences. "Animals typically respond to noise exposure by altering their usual behavior, including changes in the type and frequency of vocalization and efficiency in foraging and responding to predators," explained Martín Rocamora, one of the study researchers. "They may also develop hearing loss or increased levels of the stress hormone." For marine populations, noise pollution is amplified by overheating oceans, as warmer, more acidic water streamlines sound waves. When the natural behavior of a species is interrupted, it affects the welfare of the whole ecosystem. The National Park Service notes that with noise pollution at play, animals may not be able to communicate for mating, they may miss cues that help them avoid predators, and they may avoid noisy areas altogether, limiting their hunting grounds and habitats. The researchers have to corroborate their findings with additional insight into exactly how the generator's noise pollution affects local wildlife. However, they hope their study creates a sense of urgency on the peninsula to incorporate acoustic monitoring strategies into the management plans of Ardley Island and other ASPAs. Do you worry about air pollution in and around your home? Yes — always Yes — often Yes — sometimes No — never Click your choice to see results and speak your mind. Join our free newsletter for good news and useful tips, and don't miss this cool list of easy ways to help yourself while helping the planet.

CFIA confirms Prince Edward Island's 1st case of dermo, in oysters from Egmont Bay
CFIA confirms Prince Edward Island's 1st case of dermo, in oysters from Egmont Bay

Yahoo

time14 hours ago

  • Yahoo

CFIA confirms Prince Edward Island's 1st case of dermo, in oysters from Egmont Bay

Canadian Food Inspection Agency lab tests have confirmed the presence of the highly damaging mollusc disease dermo in oysters from P.E.I.'s Egmont Bay. "This is the first confirmed case of dermo in P.E.I.," the agency said in a news release issued Friday evening. "Dermo does not pose a risk to human health or food safety, but it can cause increased oyster mortality and decreased growth rates." In a separate email, CFIA also said that tests had confirmed the first cases of both dermo and MSX for the province of Quebec, in oyster samples collected in Chaleur Bay. It's been eight months since what was considered the first detection of dermo in Canadian waters, in oysters from Spence Cove, about five kilometres west of the Confederation Bridge in New Brunswick. Dermo disease or Perkinsosis, caused by a parasite called Perkinsus marinus, had previously been detected in Atlantic waters between Mexico and Maine. It affects both cultured and wild American oysters (Crassostrea virginica). "Outbreaks of Perkinsosis usually occur with warm summer water temperatures above 20 C," a British government fact sheet on the disease says. "Clinical signs may include pale digestive glands, gaping, shrinking of the mantle away from the outer edge of the shell and stunted growth." The CFIA news release said dermo "does not affect other bivalves in this area, including mussels, scallops or clams." Already dealing with MSX News of the positive test results from Egmont Bay will be another blow to the Island's oyster industry, which has been reeling from last July's discovery of MSX, short for multinucleate sphere unknown. That oyster disease was first detected in samples from P.E.I.'s Bedeque Bay, with more cases confirmed in other areas of the province in the months that followed. Like dermo, it affects only oysters and not the humans eating oysters from waters known to have MSX. The Canadian Food Inspection Agency says the parasite that causes dermo is easily transmitted among oysters, mostly when they are between one and three years of age. "Mortality rates are variable but are typically 50 per cent to 75 per cent," the agency says in a factsheet on its website. CFIA has power to limit movement In a statement to CBC News last month, the Canadian Food Inspection Agency acknowledged that it had been asked to test P.E.I. oyster samples for dermo, but said it had no results to share at that time. The agency did go on to outline what would happen if the samples did test positive. If a product in a PCZ [primary control zone] is deemed to be a potential risk, its movement would be controlled. — Canadian Food Inspection Agency "To prevent the spread of dermo or any other animal diseases, the CFIA may establish a primary control zone (PCZ) in areas where a disease is suspected to occur," the statement said. That kind of move would let the agency control how oysters are handled and shipped within and outside the zone. "For example, if a product in a PCZ is deemed to be a potential risk, its movement would be controlled; this means that a permit from the CFIA would be required to move this product within, through and outside of the PCZ," the statement said. "Any new PCZs would be posted as soon as possible on CFIA's website. When a new PCZ is put in place, the CFIA also notifies its provincial partners, stakeholders, Indigenous communities and industry through pre-existing communications channels."

DOWNLOAD THE APP

Get Started Now: Download the App

Ready to dive into a world of global content with local flavor? Download Daily8 app today from your preferred app store and start exploring.
app-storeplay-store